Baby Science in fin-de-siècle America

img.jpg

Founded in 1912, the U.S. Children’s Bureau established programs to expand and standardize the observation of early childhood development done by women in nurseries. Photo: Measuring board for the Children’s Bureau, circa 1920, Library of Congress, Prints and Photographs.

Baby Science in fin-de-siècle America

In her new project, Christine von Oertzen explores why members of the Association of Collegiate Alumnae in the United States came to focus in the 1880s on the early development of infants and toddlers.

Young fathers such as Darwin and French sociologist Hyppolite Taine were among the first learned men to direct their attention to newborns. On the basis of their observations, they and others produced detailed accounts of their children’s physical and psychological development. Published in 1882 by William Preyer, The Soul of a Child recorded the mental and intellectual development of Preyer’s son during the first three years of his life. Strongly influenced by Darwin’s theory of evolution, early empirical work in infant physiology and psychology charted a developmental trajectory in which language and purposeful action gradually replaced instinct and reflex. By the 1880s, investigators had stepped forward to claim that processes of physiological maturation determined psychological development in infants. A small but vocal group of male scientists were responding to Preyer’s, and Darwin’s, call to bring the nursery into the domain of academic science.

Empiricism of this kind presented science with uncommon obstacles. The intimate space of the nursery, widely regarded by contemporaries as a quintessentially female domain, restricted men’s access to human offspring.

Despite misgivings about whether women were capable of overcoming “female baby worship” to grasp an infant’s development with the requisite degree of objectivity, leading male physiologists and psychologists engaged in early childhood development research came to encourage new mothers to take notes on their infants’ physical and mental growth.

In the United States, enthusiasm for scientific approaches to childhood yielded what learned contemporaries were soon describing as the Child Study Movement. Their shared objective was to map the mind of the child. The study of newborns and infants, in contrast to small children, remains a relatively unexplored dimension of this movement. Drawing on unpublished sources, my project explains why members of the Association of Collegiate Alumnae (ACA) came to focus on the early development of infants and toddlers. Founded in 1881 to encourage female graduates of America’s most prestigious women’s colleges and coeducational universities to engage in what they described as “practical educational work,” the ACA championed baby science from 1891 until the eve of the First World War. Placing science in the cradle evolved into one of the ACA’s most ambitious projects, as ACA members seized the opportunity to widen their radius beyond the confines of late-nineteenth century domesticity.

Baby Science and the Educated Woman

In 1893, Milicent Shinn, a member of the ACA’s California Branch, joined the ACA’s committee on child study; within two years, Shinn would take charge of it altogether. Shinn began observing her niece six days after her birth and continued daily observations during the next seven years. That same year, Shinn presented her initial findings at the annual meeting of the National Educational Association in Chicago. If Sarah Wiltse, correspondent to the main journal of the Child Study Movement, The Pedagogical Seminary, is to be believed, “no section [of the meeting] attracted greater interest than that devoted to the study of children.” Through her publications, Shinn emerged—in stark contrast to her female collaborators scattered across the North American continent—as a full-fledged scholarly contributor to the academic science of her era. William Preyer, professor of physiology in the German university city of Jena, welcomed Shinn’s observations, thanking her “for having drawn my attention to some important facts in mental development which I have not specially studied.” Unstinting in his praise, Preyer concluded that “the whole series of your notes everywhere shows the stamp of reliability so manifestly that it will be a pleasure for me to make use of them for a future edition of my book on the Soul of the Child or sooner.” In a memorable turn of phrase, Shinn contested that babies developed “first the senses and then the reason; first the object, and then the word; first handcraft and then head craft.”

Shinn’s immodest goal was to account for “how the human faculties came to be what they are.” To reach these Olympian heights, Shinn instructed ACA members across North America that their observations must be undertaken with the utmost care. Aware of the multitudinous demands young mothers faced, Shinn nonetheless insisted upon strict adherence to the highest standards of field observation. Under Shinn’s leadership, the ACA Committee on Early Childhood Development matured into a network of twenty-five at-home observers. Shinn assumed the role of primus inter pares, penning letters to women in different parts of the United States filled with interpretative advice and suggestions for further reading. Shinn collected notes that ACA members forwarded to her from across the country, using their material in her own publications to question prominent scholars’ observations. The most visible outcome of their shared enterprise was Shinn’s The Development of the Senses in the First Three Years of Childhood. In contrast to the biographical studies on infants available at that time, Shinn’s 1907 publication represented the first attempt to survey all of the published and unpublished literature in her field. “The result of this carefully collated material,” as observed in a review published in the American Journal of Psychology in 1909, “is the most systematic and complete record of the development of the senses that has yet been contributed to psychology.”

fs20_img2_20.jpg

Milicent Shinn with her object of study, Ruth, at the age of seven. In: M. Shinn, Körperliche und geistige Entwicklung eines Kindes in biografischer Darstellung. Übersetzt ins Deutsche und hg. von W. Glabbach und G. Weber, Langensalza 1905

Recalling Science Beyond the Academy

The success of Shinn’s Notes on the Development of the Senses bears witness to the productivity of the ACA’s far-flung network. From the perspective of the literature that has been passed down to us in scholarly monographs and journals, Shinn’s emerges as the sole voice in what was in fact a much larger choir of home-bound, college educated women. My analysis of unpublished archival material reveals that collaborative science, as practiced by ACA members, blurred distinctions between university and home, between expert and amateur. For decades after the ACA’s remarkable experiment, publication in specialized literatures continued to mark clear-cut boundaries between salaried labors undertaken primarily by men in university labs and offices and unpaid observation carried out mostly by women at home. This history of infant scholarship encourages us to consider what else lies beyond the formal professional networks that have come to define our understanding of science itself.

More Information

This project is part of the MPIWG Working Group “Beyond the Academy: Histories of Gender and Knowledge.” Go to Website

Click here to download the program of the Working Group from July 7-9, 2011.

Milicent Shinn: Notes on the Development of a Child, Ed. 1893. Online

German version of this Research Topic

Print version of this Research Topic

Research Topics Archive

51: The Wonders of Bodily Waste
Bathymetry model of the Strait of Gibraltar ca. 1932, Instituto Español de Oceanografía.
50: The Strait in the Cold War—Deep Science and Global Geopolitics in the Mediterranean
Andreas Ryff, Münz- und Mineralienbuch, 1594. Autograph in possession of the Basel University Library (A lambda II 46a).
49: Mountain Clamor! Resource Flows and Metal Culture in Early Modern Mining
Parades of Miners, Craftsmen, and Officials Marking the Marriage of Friedrich August II, Elector of Saxony, and Maria Josepha, Archduchess of Austria in 1719. Bergakademie Freiberg.
48: Data and Decisions in Early Modern Mines
Transcript of a Bobolink song by Ferdinand S. Mathews (1904), Field Book of Wild Birds and Their Music: A Description of the Character and Music of Birds.
47: Scientific Scores and Musical Ears: Sound Diagrams in Field Recording
School of Athens
46: Early Modern Adaptation of the Aristotelian Mechanics
better shelter
45: Refugee Housing
44: Mapping Climatology
Black Hole Merger
43: One Hundred Years of Gravitational Waves
42: How High Is the Sea?
41: The Renewal of Einstein's Theory of Relativity in the Post-War Era
40: Do Data Have Politics?
39: From Sound to Knowledge
38: Colours and Their Context
37: Is Bigger Better
36: Rooting Language Family Trees
35: Making Genetics Human
34: Galileo's Laboratory of Ideas
33: Historicizing Big Data
32: Ancient Balances at the Nexus of Innovation and Knowledge
31: Looking at Diversity
30: How Recipes Created Knowledge in Early Modern Households
29: Metallurgy, Ballistics and Epistemic Instruments
28: Science under Scrutiny
27: The Globalization of Knowledge and its Consequences
26: Parts Unknown: Making the Familiar Strange
25: Apprehending Human Difference and Population Size
24: Endangerment and Its Consequences
23: The Equilibrium Controversy
22: Art and Knowledge in Pre-Modern Europe
21: Knowledgescapes
20: Baby Science in fin-de-siècle America
19: Let him reconquer language
18: Histories of Scientific Observation
17: On Historicizing Epistemology : an essay
16: Johann Lambert's Conversion to a Geometry of Space
15: The Uncertain Boundaries between Light and Matter
14: Every move will be recorded
13: Courting the Crafts in Qing China
12: The Concepts of Immanuel Kant's Natural Philosophy
11: Jean Piaget and the Child's Spontaneous Geometry
10: Galileo and the Others
9: Historicizing Knowledge about Human Biodiversity
8: Dreaming in and of Neurophilosophy
7: Who Were Einstein's Opponents?
6: Physiology of the piano
5: Numbering Bees
4: New Ways of Using Digital Images
3: Telling Instruments
2: Microscope Slides: An Object of the History of Science?
1: What (Good) is Historical Epistemology?